2019年翻譯資格考試中級筆譯練習(xí)題:孤兒院不利于兒童智力發(fā)展
漢譯英
孤兒院不利于兒童智力發(fā)展
心理學(xué)家一直認(rèn)為,在孤兒院這種機(jī)構(gòu)長大的孩子,智力的發(fā)展會受到影響,但是他們從未找到直接的證據(jù)來證明這一點(diǎn),F(xiàn)在,他們找到了。在羅馬尼亞,進(jìn)行了一項(xiàng)很特別的實(shí)驗(yàn),歷經(jīng)數(shù)年,對家庭領(lǐng)養(yǎng)和機(jī)構(gòu)收養(yǎng)兩種方式對孩子智力的影響進(jìn)行了比較研究。
該研究發(fā)現(xiàn),被領(lǐng)養(yǎng)的稚童,4歲時(shí)平均智商明顯高于在孤兒院呆了4年的同齡兒童,而且差距很大——高達(dá)8點(diǎn)。研究還表明,孩子越早被家庭領(lǐng)養(yǎng),智商越高,而2歲后才從孤兒院進(jìn)入家庭的,雖然情況因人而異,但一般而言,智力發(fā)展不大。但是,這兩組兒童的智商均明顯低于由親生父母撫養(yǎng)的一個(gè)對照組。
有些發(fā)展心理學(xué)家對此項(xiàng)研究及其贊助人提出了尖銳的批評,認(rèn)為他們研究的問題,答案是不言自明的。但以前對孤兒院收養(yǎng)和家庭領(lǐng)養(yǎng)的對比實(shí)驗(yàn)本身都存在嚴(yán)重缺陷,主要在于,誰也不知道那些進(jìn)孤兒院的孩子與進(jìn)家庭的孩子是否有什么不為人知的差異。專家稱,通過這次新的研究,人們將不再懷疑機(jī)構(gòu)收養(yǎng)的弊端,——而且能促使人們趕緊在法律允許的國家領(lǐng)養(yǎng)孩子。
“多數(shù)人憑直覺就知道,生活在家庭里比生活在孤兒院好,”威斯康辛大學(xué)一位沒有參加過此項(xiàng)研究的心理學(xué)家說道!暗渌麌业恼⒉幌矚g別人教他們?nèi)绾胃鶕?jù)直覺處理政策問題!薄斑@研究重要性在于,”他認(rèn)為,“它給出了客觀數(shù)據(jù),說明如果你同意跨國領(lǐng)養(yǎng),那么就請加快進(jìn)度,讓孩子們快些進(jìn)入家庭。”近年,許多國家,包括羅馬尼亞,禁止或嚴(yán)格限制美國家庭領(lǐng)養(yǎng)當(dāng)?shù)貎和。而在其他國家,領(lǐng)養(yǎng)手續(xù)可以拖上好幾個(gè)月。2006年,美國人共從海外領(lǐng)養(yǎng)了20679名兒童,其中超過一半來自中國、危地馬拉和俄羅斯。
90年代末,研究人員接觸過羅馬尼亞官員,希望在那里進(jìn)行些研究。當(dāng)時(shí)羅馬尼亞正在設(shè)法改善孤兒院的環(huán)境。90年代早期用條件極差的倉庫收容棄兒的做法,曾讓羅馬尼亞的孤兒院臭名遠(yuǎn)揚(yáng)。研究人員獲準(zhǔn)開始追蹤觀察136個(gè)棄嬰。他們對孩子做成長測試,然后隨機(jī)抽取,有些送往布加勒斯特的六大孤兒院收養(yǎng),有些則送往經(jīng)過仔細(xì)篩選的領(lǐng)養(yǎng)家庭。能為孩子提供“高質(zhì)量的照料服務(wù)”。
在54個(gè)月后進(jìn)行的智商測試中,被家庭領(lǐng)養(yǎng)的孩子平均智商為81,而繼續(xù)留在孤兒院的孩子則為73。研究者發(fā)現(xiàn),最早被家庭領(lǐng)養(yǎng)的孩子智力成長幅度最大。而對照組中在親生家庭成長的同齡兒童,平均智商達(dá)到109!叭澜绮煌膰摇⒁约巴粐也煌臋C(jī)構(gòu)和環(huán)境,都千差萬別,”“但我想從這些研究結(jié)果中可以把它們分為許多不同的種類,包括收養(yǎng)機(jī)構(gòu)、暴力家庭,或者不合適的領(lǐng)養(yǎng)家庭!
在設(shè)定研究計(jì)劃時(shí),研究人員提出了一個(gè)關(guān)于把孩子關(guān)到孤兒院的倫理問題,這種做法被疑對孩子有害!叭绻紤]用其他方案替代孤兒院收容制度,那說明政府已經(jīng)認(rèn)定替代方案優(yōu)于現(xiàn)行制度!毖芯空邆冞@樣寫道。
專家認(rèn)為,機(jī)構(gòu)收容中的任何共同因素都可能有延緩或阻礙智力發(fā)展的作用,包括嚴(yán)厲的控制和對孩子個(gè)人習(xí)慣和需求差異的漠視。最糟糕的是,孩子們和護(hù)理員接觸非常有限。
在一些孤兒院里,一位護(hù)理員要同時(shí)照顧20多個(gè)孩子。這些證據(jù)說明,孩子們需要及時(shí)的關(guān)愛,需要大人理解他們獨(dú)特的需求,知道他們何時(shí)饑餓何時(shí)痛苦,待他們足夠大了,會放手讓他們四處亂爬亂摸,并確保他們的安全。
參考譯文
Orphanages Stunt Mental Growth By BENEDICT CAREY
Psychologists have long believed that growing up in an institution like an orphanage stunts children's mental development but have never had direct evidence to back it up. Now they do, from an extraordinary years-long experiment in Romania that compared the effects of foster care with those of institutional child-rearing.
The study found that toddlers placed in foster families developed significantly higher I. Q.'s by age , on average, than peers who spent those years in an orphanage. The difference was large - eight points - and the study found that the earlier children joined a foster family, the better they did. Children who moved from institutional care to families after age made few gains on average, though the experience varied from child to child. Both groups, however, had significantly lower I. Q.'s than a comparison group of children raised by their biological families.
Some developmental psychologists had sharply criticized the study and its sponsor for researching a question whose answer seemed obvious. But previous attempts to compare institutional and foster care suffered from serious flaws, mainly because no one knew whether children who landed in orphanages were different in unknown ways from those in foster care.
Experts said the new study should put to rest any doubts about the harmful effects of Institutionalization _ and might help speed up adoptions from countries that still allow them. " Most of us take it as almost intuitive that being in a family is better for humans than being in an orphanage," said a psychologist at the University of Wisconsin, who was not involved in the research. "But other governments don't like to be told how to handle policy issues based on intuition." "What makes this study important," he went on, "is that it gives objective data to say that if you're going to allow international adoptions, then it's a good idea to speed things up and get kids into families quickly. In recent years many countries, including Romania, have banned or sharply restricted American families from adopting local children. In other countries, adoption procedures can drag on for many months. In 2006, Americans adopted 20,679 children from abroad, more than half of them from China, Guatemala and Russia.
The researchers approached Romanian officials in the late 1990s about conducting the study.
The country had been working to improve conditions at its orphanages, which became infamous in the early 1990s as Dickensian warehouses for abandoned children. After gaining clearance from the government, the researchers began to track 136 children who had been abandoned at birth.
They administered developmental tests to the children, and then randomly assigned them to continue at one of Bucharest's six large orphanages, or join an adoptive family. The foster families were carefully screened and provided "very high-quality care".
On I. Q. tests taken at 54, months, the foster children scored an average of 81, compared to 73 among the children who continued in an institution .The children who moved into foster care at the youngest ages tended to show the most improvement, the researchers found. The comparison group of youngsters who grew up in their biological families had an average I. Q. of 109 at the same age. "Institutions and environments vary enormously across the world and within countries," "but I think these findings generalize to many situations, from kids in institutions to those in abusive households and even bad foster care arrangements." In setting up the study, the researchers directly addressed the ethical issue of assign/ng children to institutional care, which was suspected to be harmful. "If a government is to consider alternatives to institutional care for abandoned children, it must know how the alternative compares to the standard care it provides." they wrote.
Any number of factors common to institutions could work to delay or blunt intellectual development, experts say: the regimentation, the indifference to individual differences in children's habits and needs; and most of all, the limited access to caregivers, who in some institutions can be responsible for more than 20 children at a time. The evidence seems to say that for humans, kids need a lot of responsive care giving, an adult who recognizes their distinct cry, knows when they're hungry or in pain, and gives them the opportunity to crawl around and handle different things, safely, when they're ready.
熱點(diǎn)關(guān)注:2018年翻譯考試catti二級筆譯試題(100篇)
報(bào)名時(shí)間 | 報(bào)名流程 | 考試時(shí)間 |
報(bào)考條件 | 考試科目 | 考試級別 |
成績查詢 | 考試教材 | 考點(diǎn)名錄 |
合格標(biāo)準(zhǔn) | 證書管理 | 備考指導(dǎo) |
初級會計(jì)職稱中級會計(jì)職稱經(jīng)濟(jì)師注冊會計(jì)師證券從業(yè)銀行從業(yè)會計(jì)實(shí)操統(tǒng)計(jì)師審計(jì)師高級會計(jì)師基金從業(yè)資格期貨從業(yè)資格稅務(wù)師資產(chǎn)評估師國際內(nèi)審師ACCA/CAT價(jià)格鑒證師統(tǒng)計(jì)資格從業(yè)
一級建造師二級建造師二級建造師造價(jià)工程師土建職稱公路檢測工程師建筑八大員注冊建筑師二級造價(jià)師監(jiān)理工程師咨詢工程師房地產(chǎn)估價(jià)師 城鄉(xiāng)規(guī)劃師結(jié)構(gòu)工程師巖土工程師安全工程師設(shè)備監(jiān)理師環(huán)境影響評價(jià)土地登記代理公路造價(jià)師公路監(jiān)理師化工工程師暖通工程師給排水工程師計(jì)量工程師
人力資源考試教師資格考試出版專業(yè)資格健康管理師導(dǎo)游考試社會工作者司法考試職稱計(jì)算機(jī)營養(yǎng)師心理咨詢師育嬰師事業(yè)單位教師招聘理財(cái)規(guī)劃師公務(wù)員公選考試招警考試選調(diào)生村官
執(zhí)業(yè)藥師執(zhí)業(yè)醫(yī)師衛(wèi)生資格考試衛(wèi)生高級職稱執(zhí)業(yè)護(hù)士初級護(hù)師主管護(hù)師住院醫(yī)師臨床執(zhí)業(yè)醫(yī)師臨床助理醫(yī)師中醫(yī)執(zhí)業(yè)醫(yī)師中醫(yī)助理醫(yī)師中西醫(yī)醫(yī)師中西醫(yī)助理口腔執(zhí)業(yè)醫(yī)師口腔助理醫(yī)師公共衛(wèi)生醫(yī)師公衛(wèi)助理醫(yī)師實(shí)踐技能內(nèi)科主治醫(yī)師外科主治醫(yī)師中醫(yī)內(nèi)科主治兒科主治醫(yī)師婦產(chǎn)科醫(yī)師西藥士/師中藥士/師臨床檢驗(yàn)技師臨床醫(yī)學(xué)理論中醫(yī)理論